Full-text resources of PSJD and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


Preferences help
enabled [disable] Abstract
Number of results

Journal

2010 | 59 | 3-4 | 459-466

Article title

Komunikacja i sygnalizacja zwierząt

Authors

Content

Title variants

EN
Communication and signalling in animals

Languages of publication

PL EN

Abstracts

EN
Communication involves two individuals, a sender and a receiver. In true communication, transfer of information is beneficial to the sender and access to information is beneficial to the receiver. The information transmitted in this way is the signal. Considering mechanistic limitations, the character of signalling depends on the level of conflict between the sender and receiver (if such conflict occurs) and the mechanism stabilizing the reliability of the signal in evolutionary scale. Communication is stable only if strategies of the sender and receiver reach signaling equilibrium. Signal reliability limits signal functionality. Such limitations are generally described as costs of signaling and are measured as the decrease in fitness with the increase in signal intensity, assuming that the quality of the sender and receiver are constant. Costs stabilizing the reliability of signals are strategic costs and they can be divided into two general categories: receiver-dependent and receiver-independent costs. The first are inseparable qualities of signals, the second arise as a consequence of signaling. The cost, which is necessary to convey the information unambiguously, is not a cost of signaling. The way in which the costs are paid is a function of the form of the signal. There are signals with high strategic cost, which either directly handicap the sender or charge the sender necessarily with the costly consequences of its behavior. Moreover, there are signals with negative strategic cost, i.e. displays that confer some other advantage to the sender, and signals devoid of strategic costs.

Keywords

Journal

Year

Volume

59

Issue

3-4

Pages

459-466

Physical description

Dates

published
2010

Contributors

author
  • Zakład Ekologii Behawioralnej, Instytut Biologii Środowiska, Wydział Biologii Uniwersytet im. Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Umultowska 89, 61-614 Poznań, Polska

References

  • Adams E., Mesterton-Gibbons M., 1995. The cost of threat displays and the stability of deceptive communication. J. Theor. Biol. 175, 405-421.
  • Anderson M., 1986. Evolution of condition-dependent sex ornaments and mating preferences: sexual selection based on viability differences. Evolution 40, 804-816.
  • Bell G., 1978. Handicap principle in sexual selection. Evolution 32, 872-885.
  • Bergstrom C. T., Lachmann M., 1998. Signaling among relatives. III. Talk is cheap. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 5100-5105.
  • Bradbury J. W., Vehrencamp S. L., 1998. Principles of animal communication. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland.
  • Charnov E. L., Krebs J. R., 1975. The evoution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation? Am. Nat. 109, 107-112.
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Albon S. D., 1979. The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertising. Behaviour 69, 145-170.
  • Davies N., 1981. Calling as an ownership convention on pied wagtail territories. Anim. Behav. 29, 529-534.
  • Davies N. B., Halliday T. R., 1978. Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature 274, 683-285.
  • Dawkins M. S., Guilford T., 1991. The corruption of honest signalling. Anim. Behav. 41, 865-873.
  • Dawkins R., Krebs J. R., 1978. Animal signals: information or manipulation? [W:] Behavioural Ecology. Krebs J. R., Davies A. C. (red.). Blackwell, Oxford, 282-309.
  • Dusenbery D. B., 1992. Sensory ecology : how organisms acquire and respond to information. W.H. Freeman, New York.
  • Enquist M., 1985. Communication during aggressive interactions with particular reference to variation in choice of behavior. Anim. Behav. 33, 1152-1161.
  • Evans M. R., Thomas A. L. R., 1992. The aerodynamic and mechanical effect of elongated tails in the scarlet-tufted malachite sunbird: measuring the cost of a handicap. Anim. Behav. 43, 337-347.
  • Fitch T. W., Hauser M. D., 2003. Unpacking 'Honesty': Vertebrate Vocal Production and the Evolution of Acoustic Signals. [W:] Acoustic Communication. Simmons A., Fay R. R., Popper A. N. (red.). Springer, New York, 65-137.
  • Godfray H. C. J., 1991. Signalling of need by offspring to their parents. Nature 352, 328-330.
  • Grafen A., 1990. Biological signals as handicaps. J. Theor. Biol. 144, 517-546.
  • Gray D. A., 1997. Female house crickets, Acheta domesticus, prefer the chirps of large males. Anim. Behav. 54, 1553-1562.
  • Green S., Marler P. M., 1979. The analysis of animal communication. [W:] Social Behavior and Communicatio. Marler P., Vandebergh J. G. (red.). Plenum Press, New York, 73-158.
  • Guilford T., Dawkins M. S., 1995. What are conventional signals? Anim. Behav. 49, 1689-1695.
  • Hack M. A., 1998. The Energetics of Male Mating Strategies in Field Crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllinae: Gryllidae). J. Insect Behav. 11, 853-867.
  • Hailman J. P., 1977. Optical signals : animal communication and light. Indiana University Press, Bloomington.
  • Hasson O., 1994. Cheating signals. J. Theor. Biol. 167, 223-238.
  • Hasson O., 1997. Towards a general theory of biological signaling. J. Theor. Biol. 185, 139-156.
  • Hasson O., 2000. Knowledge, information, biases and signal assemblages. [W:] Animal signals. Espmark Y., Amundsen T., Rosenqvist G. (red.). Tapir Academic Press, Trondheim, 445-463.
  • Hauser M. D., 1992. Costs of deception: cheaters are punished in rhesus monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad.Sci. USA 89, 12137-12139.
  • Hauser M. D., 1996. The evolution of communication. MIT Press, Cambridge.
  • Hauser M. D., 1997. Minding the behavior of deception. [W:] Machiavellian Intelligence II. Whiten A., Byrne R. W. (red.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 112-143.
  • Hauser M. D., Marler P., 1993. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behav. Ecol. 4, 206-212.
  • Hinde R. A., 1981. Animal signals: ethological and games-theory approaches are not incompatible. Anim. Behav. 29, 535-542.
  • Hockett C. F., Altman S. A., 1968. A not on design features. [W:] Animal Communication. Sebeok T. A. (red.). Indiana University Press, Bloomington, 61-72.
  • Hurd P. L., 1997. Is signalling of fighting ability costlier for weaker individuals? J. Theor. Biol. 184, 83-88.
  • Hurd P. L., Enquist M., 1998. Conventional signalling in aggressive interactions: the importance of temporal structure. J. Theor. Biol. 192, 197-211.
  • Iwasa Y., Pomianowski A., Nee S., 1991. The evolution of costly mate preferences. II. The 'handicap' principle. Evolution 45, 1431-1442.
  • Johnson-Laird P. N., 1990. What is communication? An introduction. [W:] Communication: Fourth Darwin Lecture Series. Mellor H. (red.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1-13.
  • Johnstone R. A., 1997. The evolution of animal signals. [W:] Behavioural Ecology. Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. (red.). Blackwell, Oxford, 155-178.
  • Johnstone R. A., 1998. Conspiratorial whispers and conspicuous displays: games of dignal detection. Evolution 52, 1554-1563.
  • Johnstone R. A., Grafen A., 1992. The continuous Sir Philip Sidney game: A simple model of biological signalling. J. Theor. Biol. 156, 215-234.
  • Johnstone R. A., Grafen A., 1993. Dishonesty and the handicap principle. Anim. Behav. 46, 759-764.
  • Kimura D., 1993. Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication. Oxford psychology series no. 20. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
  • Kokko H., 1997. Evolutionary stable strategies of age-dependent sexual advertisment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 41, 99-107.
  • Krebs J. R., Davies N. B., 1997. Behavioural ecology : an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Science, Cambridge.
  • Krebs J. R., Dawkins R., 1984. Animal signals: Mind reading and manipulation. [W:] Behavioural Ecology. Krebs J. R., Davies A. C. (red.). Sinauer Associates, 380-402.
  • Lachmann M., Számadó S., Bergstrom C. T., 2001. Cost and conflict in animal signals and human language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 13189-13194.
  • Lemel J., Walin K., 1993. Status signalling, motivational condition and dominance: an experimental study in the great tit, Parus major. Anim. Behav. 45, 549-558.
  • Marler P., 1961. The logical analysis of animal communication. J. Theor. Biol. 1, 295-317.
  • Marler P., 1977. The evolution of communication. [W:] How Animals Communicate. Sebeok T. A. (red.). Indiana University Press, Bloomington, 45-70.
  • Maynard Smith J., 1976. Sexual selection and the handicap principle. J. Theor. Biol. 57, 239-242.
  • Maynard Smith J., 1982. Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  • Maynard Smith J., 1985. Sexual selection, handicaps and true fitness. J. Theor. Biol. 115, 1-8.
  • Maynard Smith J., 1991. Honest signalling: The Sir Philip Sidney game. Anim. Behav. 42, 1034-1035.
  • Maynard Smith J., 1994. Must reliable signals be costly? Anim. Behav. 47, 1115-1120.
  • Maynard Smith J., Harper D., 1995. Animal signals: Models and terminology. J. Theor. Biol. 177, 305-311.
  • Maynard Smith J., Harper D., 2003. Animal signals. Oxford series in ecology and evolution. Oxford University Press, New York.
  • Møller A. P., 1987. Social control of deception among status signalling house sparrows Passer domesticus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 20, 307-311.
  • Møller A. P., 1990. Deceptive use of alarm calls by male swallows Hirundo rustica: A new paternity guard. Behav. Ecol. 1, 1-6.
  • Mondloch C. J., 1995. Chick hunger and begging affect parental allocation of feeding in pigeons. Anim. Behav. 49, 601-613.
  • Nowicki S., Peters S., Podos J., 1998. Song learning, early nutrition and sexual selection in songbirds. Am. Zool. 38, 179-190.
  • Nowicki S., Searcy A. W., Peters S., 2002. Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the 'nutritional stress hypothesis'. J. Comp. Physiol. A 188, 1003-1014.
  • Nur N., Hasson O., 1984. Phenotypic plasticity and the handicap principle. J. Theor. Biol. 110, 275-297.
  • Otte D., 1974. Effects and Functions in the Evolution of Signaling Systems. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5, 385-417.
  • Parker G. A., 1984. Evolutionarily stable strategies. [W:] Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. (red.). Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, 30-61.
  • Popp J. W., 1987. Risk and effectiveness in the use of agonstic displays by American goldfinches. Behaviour 103, 141-156.
  • Qvarnström A., 1997. Experimentally increased badge size increases male competition and reduces male paternal care in the collared flycatcher. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1225-1231.
  • Ramer J. D., Jenssen T. A., Hurst C., 1983. Size-related variation in the advertisement call of Rana clamitans (Anura:Ranidae) and its effect on conspecific males. Copeia 1983, 141-155.
  • Ręk P., Osiejuk T. S., 2010. Sophistication and simplicity: conventional communication in a rudimentary system. Behav. Ecol. 21, 1203-1210.
  • Robertson J. G. M., 1986. Male territoriality, fighting and assessment of fighting ability in the australian frog Uperoleria rugosa. Anim. Behav. 34, 763-772.
  • Rohwer S. A., 1982. The evolution of reliable and unreliable badges of fighting ability. Am. Zool. 32, 1325-1331.
  • Searcy W. A., Nowicki S., 2005. The evolution of animal communication : reliability and deception in signaling systems. Monographs in behavior and ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton.
  • Seleey T. D., 1989. The Honey Bee Colony as a Superorganism. Am. Sci. 77, 546-553.
  • Sherman P. W., 1985. Alarm calls of Belding's ground squirrels to aerial predators: nepotism or self-preservation? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 17, 313-323.
  • Silk J B., Kaldor E., Boyd R., 2000. Cheap talk when interests conflict. Anim. Behav. 59, 423-432.
  • Számadó S., 2003. Threat Displays are not Handicaps. J. Theor. Biol. 221, 327-348.
  • Számadó S., 2008. How threat displays work: species-specific fighting techniques, weaponry and proximity risk. Anim. Behav. 76, 1455-1463.
  • Vehrencamp S. L., 2000. Handicap, index, and conventional signal elements of bird song. [W:] Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication. Espmark Y., Amundsen T., Rosenqvist G. (red.). Tapir Academic Press, Trondheim, 277-300.
  • Vehrencamp S. L., 2001. Is song-type matching a conventional signal of aggressive intentions? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 1637-1642.
  • Wagner W. E., 1989. Fighting, assessment, and frequency alteration in Blanchard's cricket frog. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 25, 429-436.
  • Wagner W. E., 1992. Deceptive or honest signalling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in call dominant frequency by male cricket frogs. Anim. Behav. 44, 449-462.
  • Wiley R. H., 1983. The evolution of communication: Information and manipulation. [W:] Communication. Halliday T. R., Slater P. J. B. (red.). W. H. Freeman, New York, 156-189.
  • Wiley R. H., 1994. Errors, exaggeration, and deception in animal communication. [W:] Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology. Real L. (red.). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 157-189
  • Wilson E. O., 1975. Sociobiology : the new synthesis. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
  • Zahavi A., 1975. Mate selection: a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53, 205-214.
  • Zahavi A., 1977. The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). J. Theor. Biol. 67, 603-605.
  • Zahavi A., 1980. Ritualization and the evolution of movement signals. Behaviour 72, 77-81.
  • Zahavi A., 1993. The fallacy of conventional signalling. Philos. T. R. Soc. B 340, 227-230.
  • Zehavi A., Zahavi A., 1997. The handicap principle: a missing piece of Darwin's puzzle. Oxford University Press, New York.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.bwnjournal-article-ksv59p459kz
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.