Full-text resources of PSJD and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


Preferences help
enabled [disable] Abstract
Number of results
2018 | 6 | 100-109

Article title

Effect of difficulty manipulation strategies on acquisition, retention and associated perceptions in fine motor coordination task learning in young school boys

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
This study investigated whether difficulty manipulation strategies affect learning in the fine motor coordination task, perceived competence (PC) and perceived difficulty (PD). Thirty -nine novices’ right - handed boys (age 11.3 ± 0.4 years; stature 147 ± 8.94 cm ; body mass 40.57 ± 0.07 kg; mean ± SD), volunteers, were assigned to either control group (CTG: no difficulty manipulation)and two experimental groups: group 1 (EG1: one -dimension difficulty manipulation) and group 2 (EG2: two- dimensions difficulty manipu lation). All protocol sessions were conducted at the same time -of-day, in which, there were three periods: familiarization, acquisition and retention phases. Moreover, two stress- conditions of darts throw were investigated (i.e.: free condition (FC) and ti me pressure condition (TPC)). Results showed significant effect between - groups (p = 0.01, η2 = 0.215) based on difficulty strategies manipulation. Analysis showed an improvement in accuracy values in retention tests for only EG1and a significant l ower coefficient of variation (p = 0.41, η2 = 0.154) compared to the CTG and EG2. Errors decrease over time for CTG in FC (p = 0.041, η2= 0.203) but not in TPC, while no significant differences in errors for EG1 and EG2 (p = 0.19, η2 = 0.911) in the two st ress -conditions. Moreover, PD was significantly different between all test - phases (p = 0.041, η2 = 0.234) for EG1 only. The one -dimension learning strategy improves retention in accuracy performances, whereas, both strategies, do not affect errors in both FC and TPC . Therefore, teachers in physical education are not encouraged to combine difficulties in learning process of a novel fine motor coordination task.

Contributors

  • High Institute of Sport and Physical Education, Sfax University, Sfax, Tunisia
author
  • High Institute of Sport and Physical Education, Sfax University, Sfax, Tunisia
  • Faculty of Sciences, Carthage University, Bizerte,Tunisia
  • High Institute of Sport and Physical Education, Sfax University, Sfax, Tunisia
author
  • High Institute of Sport and Physical Education, Sfax University, Sfax, Tunisia
author
  • High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Ksar Said, Mannouba University, Tunis, Tunisia
  • Department of Physical Education, College of Education, King Faisal Univ ersity, Al - Hufŭf, KSA

References

  • 1. Malina RM. Motor Development during Infancy and Early Childhood: Overview and Suggested Directions for Research. International Journal of Sport and Health Science 2004; 2: 50-66. doi: 10.5432/ijshs.2.50
  • 2. Lopes V P, Rodrigues LP, Maia JA, Malina RM. Motor coordination as predictor of physical activity in childhood. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2011; 21(5): 663–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2009.01027.x
  • 3. Christina RW, Bjork RA. Optimizing long-term retention and transfer. In: Druchman D, Bjork RA, editors. In the mind's eye: Enhancing human performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1991: 23- 56
  • 4. Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2005
  • 5. Frikha M, Chaâri N, Derbel MS, Elghoul Y, Zinkovsky AV, Chamari K. Acute effect of stretching modalities and time-pressure on accuracy and consistency of throwing darts among 12-13 year-old schoolboys. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 2016; 57(9): 1089-1097. doi: 10.23736/S0022-4707.16.06521-X
  • 6. Li Y, Belkin NJ. A faceted approach to conceptualizing tasks in information seeking. Information Processing & Management 2008; 44(6): 1822-1837. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2008.07.005
  • 7. Dodonova Y, Dodonov Y. Faster on easy items, more accurate on difficult ones: Cognitive ability and performance on a task of varying difficulty. Intelligence 2013; 41(1): 1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.003
  • 8. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsh B, Zarahn E, Celnik PA, Krakauer JW. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106(5): 1590–1595. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805413106.
  • 9. Frikha M, Derbel MS, Chaâri N, Gharbi A, Chamari K. Acute effect of stretching modalities on global coordination and kicking accuracy in 12–13 year-old soccer players. Human Movement Science 2017; 54: 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.03.008
  • 10. Sawers A, Hahn ME. Gradual training reduces practice difficulty while preserving motor learning of a novel locomotor task. Human Movement Science 2013; 32(4): 605-617. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.02.004
  • 11. Onushko T, Kim C, Evangelos A, Christou EA. Reducing task difficulty during practice improves motor learning in older adults. Experimental Gerontology 2014; 57: 168–174. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2014.06.006
  • 12. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, Souissi N. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res 2014; 45(5): 789-801. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2014.921409
  • 13. Lam WK., Masters RS, Maxwell JP. Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill. Consciousness and Cognition 2010; 19(4): 1058–1061. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.005
  • 14. Ong NT, Lohse KR, Hodges NJ. Manipulating target size influences perceptions of success when learning a dart-throwing skill but does not impact retention. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1378. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01378
  • 15. Zahradník, D, Vaverka F, Gajda G. Optimisation of the size of a target and the throwing distance during a throw at a target for adults. Acta Univ. Palacki. Olomuc., Gymn2008; 38(4): 39-45.
  • 16. Frömer R, Hafner V, and Sommer W. Aiming for the bull’s eye: Preparing for throwing investigated with event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology 2012; 49(3): 335-344. doi: 10.1111/j.1469- 8986.2011.01317.x
  • 17. Koyuncu M. Broadbent’s cognitive approach and its effect on motor performance in sports. Psychology 2011; 02(05): 472–476. doi:10.4236/psych.2011.25073
  • 18. Henderson SE, Sugden DA, Barnett A. Movement Assessment Battery for Children–2 examiner’s manual. 2nd ed. London England: Harcourt Assessment; 2007
  • 19. Li W, Lee A, Solmon M. The role of perceptions of task difficulty in relation to self-perceptions of ability, intrinsic value, attainment value, and performance. European Physical Education Review 2007; 13(3): 301-318. doi: 10.1177/1356336X07081797
  • 20. Bandoura A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman; 1997
  • 21. Eccles JS, Adler TF, Futterman R, Goff SB, Kaczala CM, Meece J, Midgley C. Expectancies, Values, and Academic Behaviors. In JT, Spence, editors. Achievement and Achievement Motives. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman; 1983: 75-146
  • 22. Zhang P, Wang Z, Adesope O. The Effects of Goal Type, Learning Interest, and Task Difficulty on Learning English Words. International Journal of Learning, Teaching and Educational Research 2016; 15(2): 32-46. https://www.ijlter.org/
  • 23. Brière NM, Vallerand RJ, Blais MR, Pelletier LG. Développement et Validation d'une Mesure de Motivation Intrinsèque, Extrinsèque et d'Amotivation en Contexte Sportif: L'Échelle de Motivation dans les Sports (ÉMS) [Development and validation of a scale on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and lack of motivation in sports: The Scale on Motivation in Sports]. International Journal of Sport Psychology 1995; 26(4): 465-489.
  • 24. Edwards B, Waterhouse J, Atkinson G, Reilly T. Effects of time of day and distance upon accuracy and consistency of throwing darts. J Sports Sci 2007; 25(3): 1531–1538. doi: 10.1080/02640410701244975
  • 25. Edwards B, Waterhouse J. Effects of one night of partial sleep deprivation upon diurnal rhythms of accuracy and consistency in throwing darts. ChronobiolInt 2009; 26(4): 756–768. doi:10.1080/07420520902929037
  • 26. Delignières D, Famose JP, Thépaut-Mathieu C, Fleurance P. A psychophysical study on difficulty ratings in rock climbing. Int J Sport Psychol 1993; 24: 404-416.
  • 27. Bland JM, Altman DG. Comparing methods of measurement: why plotting difference against standard method is misleading. Lancet 1995; 346(8982): 1085-1087.
  • 28. Chiviacowsky S, Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Self-controlled learning: the importance of protecting perceptions of competence. Front Psychol 2012; 3: 458. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00458
  • 29. Trempe M, Sabourin M, Proteau L. Success modulates consolidation of a visuomotor adaptation task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn 2012; 38(1): 52–60. doi:10.1037/a0024883
  • 30. Heimbeck D, Frese M, Sonnentag S, Keith N. Integrating errors into the training process: The function of error management instructions and the role of goal orientation. Personnel Psychology 2003; 56: 333– 361. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00153.x
  • 31. Capa RL, Audiffren M, Ragot S. The effects of achievement motivation, task difficulty, and goal difficulty on physiological, behavioral, and subjective effort. Psychophysiology 2008; 45(5): 859–868. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00675.x
  • 32. De Corte E. Transfer as the productive use of acquired knowledge, skills, and motivations. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2003; 12(4): 142–146. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01250
  • 33. Roediger HL, Karpicke JD. The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science 2006; 1(3): 181–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1745- 6916.2006.00012.x
  • 34. Hughes MG, Day EA, Wang X, Schuelke MJ, Arsenault ML, Harkrider LN, Cooper OD. Learner-controlled practice difficulty in the training of a complex task: cognitive and motivational mechanisms. Journal of Applied Psychology 2013; 98(1): 80-98. doi: 10.1037/a0029821
  • 35. Schmidt RA., Bjork RA. New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science 1992; 3(4): 207–217. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9280.1992.tb00029.x
  • 36. Bjork RA, Bjork EL. A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In Healy A, Kosslyn S, Shiffrin R, editors. From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992: 35–67
  • 37. Maxwell JP, Masters RS, Kerr E, Weedon E. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. Q J ExpPsychol A 2001; 54(4): 1049-68. doi:10.1080/713756014
  • 38. Holyoak KJ, Spellman BA. Thinking. Annual Review of Psychology 1993; 44: 265–315. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.001405
  • 39. Ntoumanis N, Edmunds J, Duda JL. Understanding the coping process from a self-determination theory perspective. British Journal of Health Psychology 2009; 14: 249-260. doi: 10.1348/135910708X349352
  • 40. Wood R, Bandura A. Impact of conceptions of ability on self-regulatory mechanisms and complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1989; 56(3): 407-415. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.56.3.407

Document Type

paper

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.psjd-a2fcc862-ceb6-4850-b943-530cf60054e2
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.